Detecting and responding to moralised misinformation


Matthew Andreotta1 Cécile Paris2 Ingrid van Putten1 Mark Hurlstone3 Iain Walker4 Fabio Boschetti1

1Environment, CSIRO
2Data61, CSIRO
3Department of Psychology, Lancaster University
4Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne


25 September 2023


 Email: matthew.andreotta@csiro.au

 Website: matt-lab.github.io

 LinkedIn: @matthew-andreotta


 Link to slides: matt-lab.github.io/workshop_moral-misinformation

Outline


  1. The concept of moral misinformation.
  2. A tool for detecting moral messages.
  3. An application of the tool.
  4. Potential applications for the Russo-Ukraine War.

“Now, what if these [American] biolabs are handling very dangerous pathogens that may lead to the deaths in people in the areas? There are reports of possibly deadly pathogens escaping these biolabs in places like Ukraine, Georgia, Kazakhstan, that is responsible for killing people.” (Greene, 2022)

The consequences of moral convictions

  1. License to be uncompromising
  1. License to share falsehoods

Moral Foundations Theory

Care/Harm

👼
Caring
Kindness

😈
Cruelty

Fairness/Cheating

👼
Fairness
Justice
Honesty
Trustworthiness

😈
Dishonesty

Loyalty/Betrayal

👼
Loyalty
Patriotism
Self-sacrifice

😈
Cowardice

Authority/Subversion

👼
Obedience
Deference

😈
Disobedience

Purity/Degradation

👼
Temperance
Chastity
Piety
Cleanliness

😈
Over-indulgence
Lust

An application: moralisation of the Great Barrier Reef by climate change contrarians

Photo credit: Justin Marshall

An application: moralisation of the Great Barrier Reef by climate change contrarians

Research Question

Do climate change contrarians post more moralised messages about the Great Barrier Reef than other users who post about climate change?

An application: moralisation of the Great Barrier Reef by climate change contrarians

  1. Contrarian
  • Used a contrarian phrase in a Great Barrier Reef tweet:
    • “#ClimateCult”
    • “climate hysteria”
    • “alarmist”

564 users

7,797 tweets about the Great Barrier Reef

  1. Baseline
  • Used a climate change phrase in a Great Barrier Reef tweet:
    • “climate change”
    • “#ClimateAction”
    • “#ClimateEmergency”

16,862 users

187,081 tweets about the Great Barrier Reef

An application: moralisation of the Great Barrier Reef by climate change contrarians

An application: moralisation of the Great Barrier Reef by climate change contrarians

Other applications

  • Identify moral characteristics of dis/misinformation
  • Detecting success of disinformation campaign
  • Identify users who may be predisposed to believe or share dis/misinformation

“Now, what if these [American] biolabs are handling very dangerous pathogens that may lead to the deaths in people in the areas? There are reports of possibly deadly pathogens escaping these biolabs in places like Ukraine, Georgia, Kazakhstan, that is responsible for killing people…

“What if that’s true? I think these are questions that we should ask because no american citizen wants to be held morally and ethically responsible and the U.S. government should not be funding something that’s killing people in a country that’s not even our own, let alone here at home.” (Greene, 2022)

Summary

Misinformation embedded in morals can be particularly potent

  • Moral convictions can motivate people to be uncompromising, be hostile to those with different views, and share misinformation.
  • We can detect moral messages with Natural Language Processing techniques.
  • Climate change contrarian tweets have a unique moral signature.


 Link to slides: matt-lab.github.io/workshop_moral-misinformation

 Email: matthew.andreotta@csiro.au

References

Brady, W. J., Gantman, A. P., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2020). Attentional capture helps explain why moral and emotional content go viral. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 149(4), 746–756. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000673
Brady, W. J., Jackson, J. C., Lindström, B., & Crockett, M. (2023). Algorithm-Mediated Social Learning in Online Social Networks. OSF Preprints. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/yw5ah
Brady, W. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2021). Estimating the effect size of moral contagion in online networks: A pre-registered replication and meta-analysis. OSF Preprints. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/s4w2x
Coan, T. G., Boussalis, C., Cook, J., & Nanko, M. O. (2021). Computer-assisted classification of contrarian claims about climate change. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 22320. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-01714-4
Cook, J., Nuccitelli, D., Green, S. A., Richardson, M., Winkler, B., Painting, R., … Skuce, A. (2013). Quantifying the Consensus on Anthropogenic Global Warming in the Scientific Literature. Environmental Research Letters, 8(2), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/8/2/024024
CSIRO. (2023). Emergency Situation Awareness. https://esa.csiro.au/aus/about-public.html.
Curry, O. S., Mullins, D. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2019). Is It Good to Cooperate?: Testing the Theory of Morality-as-Cooperation in 60 Societies. Current Anthropology, 60(1), 47–69. https://doi.org/10.1086/701478
Ecker, U. K. H., Lewandowsky, S., Cook, J., Schmid, P., Fazio, L. K., Brashier, N., … Amazeen, M. A. (2022). The psychological drivers of misinformation belief and its resistance to correction. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(1), 13–29. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-021-00006-y
Garrett, K. N., & Bankert, A. (2020). The Moral Roots of Partisan Division: How Moral Conviction Heightens Affective Polarization. British Journal of Political Science, 50(2), 621–640. https://doi.org/10.1017/S000712341700059X
Greene, M. T. (2022). U.S. House of Representatives: House Session. C-SPAN.
Helgason, B. A., & Effron, D. A. (2022). It might become true: How prefactual thinking licenses dishonesty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 123(5), 909–940. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000308
Hoover, J., Johnson, K., Boghrati, R., Graham, J., & Dehghani, M. (2018). Moral Framing and Charitable Donation: Integrating Exploratory Social Media Analyses and Confirmatory Experimentation. Collabra: Psychology, 4(1), 9. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.129
Kennedy, B., Golazizian, P., Trager, J., Atari, M., Hoover, J., Mostafazadeh Davani, A., & Dehghani, M. (2023). The (moral) language of hate. PNAS Nexus, 2(7), pgad210. https://doi.org/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgad210
Kodapanakkal, R. I., Brandt, M. J., Kogler, C., & van Beest, I. (2022). Moral relevance varies due to inter-individual and intra-individual differences across big data technology domains. European Journal of Social Psychology, 52(1), 46–70. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2814
Konkes, C., & Foxwell-Norton, K. (2021). Science communication and mediatised environmental conflict: A cautionary tale. Public Understanding of Science, 30(4), 470–483. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662520985134
Lamb, W. F., Mattioli, G., Levi, S., Roberts, J. T., Capstick, S., Creutzig, F., … Steinberger, J. K. (2020). Discourses of climate delay. Global Sustainability, 3, e17. https://doi.org/10.1017/sus.2020.13
Power, R., Robinson, B., & Cameron, M. (2023). Insights from a decade of Twitter monitoring for emergency management. Proceedings of the Information Systems for Crisis Response and Management Asia Pacific Conference 2022.
Power, R., Robinson, B., Colton, J., & Cameron, M. (2014). Emergency Situation Awareness: Twitter case studies. Information Systems for Crisis Response and Management in Mediterranean Countries, 218–231. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11818-5_19
Ryan, T. J. (2017). No Compromise: Political Consequences of Moralized Attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 61(2), 409–423. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12248
Skitka, L. J., Hanson, B. E., Morgan, G. S., & Wisneski, D. C. (2021). The Psychology of Moral Conviction. Annual Review of Psychology, 72(1), 347–366. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-063020-030612
Solovev, K., & Pröllochs, N. (2022). Moralized language predicts hate speech on social media. PNAS Nexus, pgac281. https://doi.org/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgac281
Spring, V. L., Cameron, C. D., & Cikara, M. (2018). The Upside of Outrage. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(12), 1067–1069. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.09.006
Većkalov, B., Geiger, S. J., White, M. P., Rutjens, B., Harreveld, F. van, Stablum, F., … Linden, D. S. van der. (2023). A 27-country test of communicating the scientific consensus on climate change. OSF Preprints. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/bctm3